Four- and Five-month-olds Can Track Briefly Occluded Objects
Evidence that four- and five-month-olds can track momentarily occluded objects comes from studies using habituation, violation-of-expectations, anticipatory looking and the completion of an action directed to a fully occluded object that was prepared in advance of the object being fully occluded.
Notes
A wide range of evidence suggests that
four- and five-month-olds can track
briefly occluded objects.
Such evidence comes from infants’ reactions to a range of different scenarios.
Some scenrios involve a comparision between the number of objects
(e.g. Spelke, Kestenbaum, Simons, & Wein, 1995),
others involve infants’ abilities to track the causal effects of
unperceived objects
(e.g. Baillargeon, 1987),
while others require infants to track properties such as the shape and
size of unperceived objects
(e.g. Wang, Baillargeon, & Brueckner, 2004),
or to remember the location of a hidden object (e.g. Wilcox, Nadel, & Rosser, 1996).
The evidence also comes from studies using a variety of different methods.
These include
habituation (e.g. Spelke et al., 1995),
violation-of-expectation (e.g. Wang et al., 2004),
and
anticipatory looking (e.g. Rosander & Hofsten, 2004; Bertenthal, Gredebäck, & Boyer, 2013).
How do infants track briefly occluded objects?
An early idea was that infants’ earliest abilities involved
knowledge of physical objects:
‘objects are conceived: Humans come to know about an object’s ... boundaries ... in ways like those by which we come
to know about its material composition or its market value.’
(Spelke, 1998, p. 198)
One prediction of this idea is that infants should be able to search for briefly occluded objects.
Because that prediction has been falsified (e.g. Shinskey & Munakata, 2001),
the idea should probably be rejected.
We therefore need alternative ideas about how infants track briefly occluded objects ...
Glossary
habituation :
Habituation is used to test hypotheses about which events are interestingly different to an infant.
In a habituation experiment, infants are shown an event repeatedly until it no longer holds their interest, as measured by how long they look at it.
The infants are then divided into two (or more) groups and each group is shown a new event.
How much longer do they look at the new event than at the most recent presentation of the old event?
This difference in looking times indicates dishabituation, or the reawakening of interest.
Given the assumption that greater dishabituation indicates that the old and new events are more interestingly different to the infant, evidence from patterns of dishabituation can sometimes support conclusions about patterns in how similar and different events are to infants.
tracking an attribute :
For a process to track an attribute or thing is for the presence or absence of the attribute or thing
to make a difference to how the process unfolds,
where this is not an accident. (And for a system or device to track an attribute is for some process
in that system or device to track it.)
Tracking an attribute or thing is contrasted with computing it.
Unlike tracking, computing typically requires that the attribute be represented.
violation-of-expectation :
Violation-of-expectation experiments test hypotheses about what infants
expect by comparing their responses to two events.
The responses compared are usually looking durations.
Looking durations are linked to infants’ expectations by the assumption
that, all things being equal, infants will typically look longer at
something which violates an expectation of theirs than something which
does not.
Accordingly, with careful controls, it is sometimes possible to draw
conclusions about infants’ expectations from evidence that they generally
look longer at one event than another.
References
Baillargeon, R. (1987). Object permanence in 3.5-and 4.5-month-old infants.
Developmental Psychology,
23(5), 655–664.
Bertenthal, B. I., Gredebäck, G., & Boyer, T. W. (2013). Differential contributions of development and learning to infants’ knowledge of object continuity and discontinuity.
Child Development,
84(2), 413–421.
https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12005
Charles, E. P., & Rivera, S. M. (2009). Object permanence and method of disappearance: Looking measures further contradict reaching measures.
Developmental Science,
12(6), 991–1006.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00844.x
Jonsson, B., & Von Hofsten, C. (2003). Infants’ ability to track and reach for temporarily occluded objects.
Developmental Science,
6(1), 86–99.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00258
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1998). Object representation, identity, and the paradox of early permanence: Steps toward a new framework.
Infant Behavior and Development,
21(2), 201–235.
Moore, M. K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Factors affecting infants’ manual search for occluded objects and the genesis of object permanence.
Infant Behavior and Development,
31(2), 168–180.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.10.006
Rosander, K., & Hofsten, C. von. (2004). Infants’ emerging ability to represent occluded object motion.
Cognition,
91(1), 1–22.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00166-5
Shinskey, J. L., & Munakata, Y. (2001). Detecting transparent barriers: Clear evidence against the means-end deficit account of search failures.
Infancy,
2(3), 395–404.
Spelke, E. S. (1988). Where perceiving ends and thinking begins: The apprehension of objects in infancy. In A. Yonas (Ed.),
Perceptual development in early infancy (pp. 197–234). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Spelke, E. S. (1998). Nativism, empiricism, and the origins of knowledge.
Infant Behavior and Development,
21(2), 181–200.
Spelke, E. S., Kestenbaum, R., Simons, D. J., & Wein, D. (1995). Spatiotemporal continuity, smoothness of motion and object identity in infancy.
British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
13(2), 113–142.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1995.tb00669.x
Wang, S., Baillargeon, R., & Brueckner, L. (2004). Young infants’ reasoning about hidden objects: Evidence from violation-of-expectation tasks with test trials only.
Cognition,
93(3), 167–198.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2003.09.012
Wilcox, T., Nadel, L., & Rosser, R. (1996). Location memory in healthy preterm and full-term infants.
Infant Behavior and Development,
19(3), 309–323.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(96)90031-4
Wynn, K. (1992). Addition and subtraction by human infants.
Nature,
358(6389), 749–750.
https://doi.org/10.1038/358749a0